The Science of Attachment: How Early Bonds Influence Our Lifelong Responses

Zee
8 min readSep 30, 2024

--

To see an illustration of this article, you can watch this video

Imagine a newborn, eyes wide, just beginning to make sense of the world. When something frightening happens, like a loud noise or a stranger approaching, this child instinctively looks for comfort — most often in their caregiver's arms. This act of seeking closeness and reassurance is more than just a physical response; it’s deeply wired into our biology, something John Bowlby (1969) called attachment — a state where our biological systems are intertwined with our psychological needs for safety and security.

As these tiny humans grow, usually around 6 to 9 months, they start showing obvious signs of this attachment system in action. Imagine a baby, startled by something unfamiliar, quickly crawling toward their caregiver. It’s not just any random movement — it’s a hardwired behavioral response meant to protect and soothe them (Marvin & Britner, 1999). Behind this seemingly simple action, their little hearts slow and their minds settle, knowing they are safe.

Now, picture that child years later as an adult. Their memories of being comforted, held, or ignored have now become stories that shape how they relate to others. According to Gander and Buchheim (2015), early attachment forms mental representations that govern how we handle emotions, how we remember events, and even how reactive our nervous system becomes when we’re stressed. But there’s a spectrum of how secure or insecure these bonds are, and researchers have developed tools to understand them.

For infants, a famous method called the Strange Situation (SS) procedure helps classify how secure their attachments are. Imagine a scenario where a baby is briefly separated from their caregiver, only to reunite moments later, how that baby responds — whether by quickly calming down or remaining upset — reveals much about their inner attachment framework.

A study involving infants aged 12–20 months categorized them based on their attachment type to understand these dynamics better.

  1. Around six months of age, infants start to anticipate specific reactions from their caregivers when distressed and adjust their behavior accordingly. For example, they may develop coping strategies based on their daily caregiver interactions. Infants whose caregivers consistently respond to distress in a sensitive and affectionate way — such as immediately picking them up and comforting them — feel secure knowing they can openly express negative emotions and receive comfort. Their coping strategies are considered ‘organized’ and ‘secure’ because they seek closeness and maintain contact with their caregiver until they feel safe. The strategy is called ‘organized’ because the child knows exactly what to do with a responsive caregiver — approach them when distressed.
  2. On the other hand, infants whose caregivers consistently respond insensitively or rejectingly — by ignoring, teasing, or reacting negatively — develop coping strategies that are also ‘organizedbut insecure. These children avoid their caregiver when distressed and minimize expressions of negative emotions. This avoidance is considered an ‘organized’ strategy because the child knows how to respond to a rejecting caregiver by avoiding them when needed, but it is ‘insecure’ as it raises the risk of emotional and behavioral issues.
  3. Infants whose caregivers respond unpredictably or inconsistently — such as expecting the infant to meet their needs or amplifying the infant’s distress — develop another ‘organized’ strategy. They display exaggerated negative emotions to capture the caregiver’s attention. This behavior is ‘organized’ because the child knows how to deal with an inconsistently responsive caregiver — showing extreme distress and frustration, hoping that their intense emotions will prompt a response. However, this resistant strategy is also ‘insecure’ because it increases the risk of emotional and social maladjustment.
  4. Disorganized attachment occurs in about 15% of infants from low-risk environments and up to 82% of infants in high-risk situations. These infants do not use any of the three organized coping strategies. Disorganized attachment often results from atypical caregiver behavior — frightening, detached, or unusual interactions — which is not limited to times when the infant is distressed. Caregivers exhibiting these behaviors often have unresolved grief, trauma, or histories of emotional, physical, or sexual abuse.
    Disorganized infants face a unique dilemma: their source of safety (the caregiver) is also the cause of their fear and distress. This makes the typical ‘organized’ strategies ineffective in restoring their sense of security.

But what about adults? Well, that’s where things get a bit more complex. For adults, two major methods are used: self-report measurements, which rely on an individual’s conscious self-evaluation, and narrative methods, where individuals share early childhood memories and reflect on them from their current perspectives. One famous tool, the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI), classifies adults into four categories of attachment:

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=WjOowWxOXCg

1. Secure-autonomous Attachment

Secure-autonomous individuals — tell stories about their caregivers that show balance. They can reflect on both the good and the bad in their relationships, creating a coherent narrative. In their lives, they are like explorers with an “internal secure base,” able to delve into their thoughts and feelings without fear.

Secure attachment typically forms when a caregiver is consistently responsive, warm, and emotionally available. When children experience a predictable and comforting environment, they learn to trust that their needs will be met. For example, a mother who responds quickly and sensitively to her baby’s cries, providing both emotional comfort and physical needs, fosters secure attachment.

In childhood, securely attached children tend to explore their environment confidently, knowing they can return to their caregiver for comfort and safety. And when in adulthood, these individuals often form healthy, trusting relationships, where they can communicate openly and handle conflicts constructively. They also tend to be more emotionally resilient and have balanced self-esteem.

2. Insecure-dismissing Attachment

Insecure-dismissing individuals — often downplay the importance of early relationships or idealize them. Their narratives might seem distant, more focused on achievements than emotional connection. Their attachment relationships, though functional, often feel emotionally absent.

Insecure-dismissing attachment in adults stems from a history where early caregivers minimized or rejected emotional experiences. This is common when parents emphasize independence to the point where emotional needs are ignored. For example, a child whose parents focus on achievements and discourage emotional displays (e.g., a father who tells his child to “toughen up” when they cry) may learn to suppress their emotions to gain approval.

In adulthood, these individuals often dismiss the importance of close relationships, de-emphasize emotional expression, and may struggle to understand their own or others’ emotions. Their relationships might feel emotionally distant or devoid of deeper connections.

3. Insecure-preoccupied Attachment

Insecure-preoccupied individuals — by contrast, get stuck in their memories of caregivers. Their stories brim with unresolved emotions, often marked by frustration, and they’re more focused on past wounds than present realities.

Insecure-preoccupied attachment in adults is often rooted in inconsistent caregiving during childhood, where parents were sometimes available and sometimes neglectful. These individuals develop an over-reliance on relationships for validation and stability.

People with this type of attachment whose parents were overly controlling or emotionally intrusive might have grown up feeling emotionally smothered, but they still long for their approval and attention. These individuals often become overly dependent on their romantic partners for validation and self-worth. They tend to feel anxious about their relationships, fearing abandonment, and may exhibit controlling behaviors to prevent loss.

4. Unresolved/Fearful Attachment

Then, there are the unresolved individuals — who have faced trauma, like abuse or loss. In their stories, coherence breaks down. They’re overwhelmed, often speaking in ways that reflect confusion or fear, unable to organize their experiences into a clear narrative. Caregivers in these situations may have been emotionally or physically abusive, leaving the child with unprocessed emotions and a deep sense of insecurity.

An individual who was neglected or abused by their caregiver might grow up with a deep-seated fear of closeness, as relationships trigger memories of trauma. Those with unresolved attachment may have intense fears of being hurt in relationships, leading them to avoid closeness or develop unstable, chaotic relationships. They often have difficulty trusting others and may swing between extreme emotional reactions in their relationships.

Source: https://neurogenbb.com/what-are-brain-waves/

So why does this matter? Why should we care about whether someone is secure, dismissing, preoccupied, or unresolved?

The answer lies in the body’s response to stress. Secure attachment acts like a buffer against life’s inevitable storms. Those who are securely attached can balance exploration with attachment, open up emotionally, and use loved ones as a safe harbor. But for the insecurely attached, emotional regulation can be a struggle. They either suppress emotions (avoidant) or become overwhelmed by them (preoccupied), leading to higher stress reactivity.

The region in charge of motion in the brain and the basic characteristics of the left and right hemispheres (https://doi.org/10.3390/app9224885)

One fascinating insight comes from studies of EEG brain waves, which reveal a connection between attachment and how the brain handles emotions. The left side of the brain, it seems, is more active when we’re in a positive emotional state and ready to approach others, while the right side lights up when we feel distressed or withdrawn (Fox & Davidson, 1984). Secure individuals show more balance between these two sides, able to flexibly regulate their emotions. However insecure individuals, especially those with unresolved attachments, often struggle, showing heightened physiological responses to stress.

And then there’s the matter of the heart — the literal heart. Studies like those by Sroufe and Waters (1977) tell a remarkable story: babies who are securely attached calm down quickly after reuniting with their caregivers. Their heart rates slow as if their bodies instinctively know they are safe again. But insecurely attached babies, especially those with avoidant tendencies, struggle to return to a state of calm, even minimizing their emotional responses to protect themselves from stress.

Even as adults, this physiological dance continues. Secure individuals, when asked to recall stressful events, show lower levels of skin conductance — a measure of how much they’re sweating due to stress. Meanwhile, those with dismissing or preoccupied styles show heightened responses, their bodies reacting more strongly to memories of loss or rejection (Dozier & Kobak, 1992). It’s as if their bodies are still waiting for the safety that never came.

Attachment isn’t just a psychological phenomenon — it’s deeply embedded in our biology. From the way our brains process emotion to the way our hearts beat, these early bonds shape our lifelong responses to love, loss, and connection.

References

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss: Volume I. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
  • Dozier, M., & Kobak, R. R. (1992). Psychophysiology in attachment interviews: Converging evidence for deactivating strategies. Child Development, 63(6), 1473–1480.
  • Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1984). Hemispheric substrates of affect: A developmental model. In N. A. Fox & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The psychobiology of affective development (pp. 281–298). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Gander, M., & Buchheim, A. (2015). Attachment representations, psychodynamic treatment, and relationship satisfaction in couples: A mediator-modelling approach. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Practice, 88(3), 290–304.
  • Marvin, R. S., & Britner, P. A. (1999). Normative development: The ontogeny of attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications (pp. 44–67). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational construct. Child Development, 48(4), 1184–1199.

--

--

Zee
Zee

Written by Zee

I captured each moment through the art of writing

No responses yet